Menu Close
By attaching a small transponder to bees, researchers were able to track movements and feeding patterns. Andrew Martin

Fast food: how bees navigate to nectar, without the bumbling

How do insects, given their poor visual resolution and small brains, cope with the huge challenge of finding food at more than one location and returning home immediately afterwards, day after day?

Mathieu Lihoreau and colleagues from Queen Mary University of London have come up with a very clever experimental design to answer this question in bumblebees (Bombus terrestris) – and their study is published today in PLoS Biology.

We knew already that animals of all varieties use a range of navigational information available in the environment. That ranges from using the sun as a compass, the earth’s magnetic field, distinct visual landmarks in the panorama and occasionally even scent.

But food sources animals rely on often get replenished on a daily basis (e.g. nectar in flowers or ripened fruits on a tree). Hence animals that use such food sources have to develop robust strategies to find multiple food locations, while always knowing where home is.

In the first part of the experiment, flowers were position at points 1-5 in the image above. In the second part of the experiment, the flower in location 3 was moved to location 6. CLICK FOR LARGER VIEW. PLOS Biology/Mathieu Lihoreau et al

A closer look at the study

Working in the pasture lands of Rothamsted estate, UK, Lihoreau and colleagues trained bees to collect sugary solutions from artificial flowers. After training, the researchers then tested individual bees by arranging the artificial flowers in a pentagonal shape (see image above).

Tracking insects, especially flying insects, in an outdoor environment has been a huge challenge. Here, the authors caught individual bees that were leaving the hive and attached a small transponder (weighing 0.8mg) and released them.

The coordinates (latitude and longitude) of the tagged bee were then tracked using a harmonic radar which provided an accurate flight path for each bee.

With increasing experience, bees not only visited more flowers before returning to hive, but the sequence in which they visited the five flowers also become more similar.

Bees very quickly then determined the shortest possible route from one flower to another. The radar tracks of the bees (see image below) clearly show that flight paths are very straight, both between the nest to the flower and between flowers.

The distance to travel the shortest possible path to visit the five flowers was about 310m. Inexperienced bees travelled much greater distances (about 1,950m) to visit the same five flowers, whereas the experienced individuals travelled only about 460m – much closer to the shortest possible route.

Path A is that taken by an untrained bee. Paths B-F are those taken by experienced bees toward the end of their training. CLICK FOR LARGER VIEW. PLOS Biology/Mathieu Lihoreau et al

Optimal strategy

Using optimisation algorithms the authors propose the bees find the optimal strategy of taking the shortest route by a simple routine that improves over successive trips. If Lihoreau and colleagues are right, a bee compares the length of its current route to the total length of the shortest route it has experienced up until that point.

Using that experience the bee chooses to re-use the shortest flight paths.

When the researchers altered the spatial configuration of the flowers (one flower was removed and placed elsewhere) from the earlier pentagon shape, bees continued to maintain the same flight sequence they had previously. This meant the bees would visit the location of the now-absent flower.

Since the bees did not get enough food along one flight trip, they flew twice in the same sequence before returning to the hive!

This study shows how bees venture into previously unchartered territories to find new flowers with nectar. It also nicely shows that each individual bee quickly determines the shortest possible path along a route with multiple destinations.

The humble bumblebee might have a tiny brain but it’s still capable of some impressive navigation and food-tracking techniques.

Needless to say, this new paper should create a buzz.

Want to write?

Write an article and join a growing community of more than 182,300 academics and researchers from 4,941 institutions.

Register now